Mind Matters |
Mind & Brain
Empathy's surprising roots in the sense of touch
By Jakub Limanowski
|
June 26, 2012 |
Do you feel “touched” by the plight of another?
Image: iStock/Valua Vitaly
When a friend hits her thumb with a hammer, you don't have to put much
effort into imagining how this feels. You know it immediately. You will
probably tense up, your "Ouch!" may arise even quicker than your
friend's, and chances are that you will feel a little
pain
yourself. Of course, you will then thoughtfully offer consolation and
bandages, but your initial reaction seems just about automatic. Why?
Neuroscience now offers you an answer: A recent line of research has
demonstrated that seeing other people being touched activates primary
sensory areas of your brain, much like experiencing the same touch
yourself would do. What
these findings suggest is beautiful in its simplicity—that you literally "feel with" others.
There is no denying that the exceptional
interpersonal understanding we
humans show is by and large a product of our emotional responsiveness.
We are automatically affected by other people’s feelings, even without
explicit communication. Our involvement is sometimes so powerful that we
have to flee it, turning our heads away when we see someone get hurt in
a movie. Researchers hold that this capacity emerged
long before humans evolved.
However, only quite recently has it been given a name: A mere hundred
years ago, the word "Empathy"—a combination of the Greek "in"
(em-) and "feeling" (
pathos)—was coined by the British psychologist
E. B. Titchener during his endeavor to translate the German
Einfühlungsvermögen ("the ability to feel into").
Despite the lack of a universally agreed-upon definition of empathy, the mechanisms of
sharing and understanding another’s experience have always been of scientific and public interest—and particularly so since the introduction of
“mirror neurons.”
This important discovery was made two decades ago by Giacomo
Rizzolatti and his co-workers at the University of Parma, who were
studying motor neuron properties in macaque monkeys. To compensate for
the tedious electrophysiological recordings required, the monkey was
occasionally given food rewards. During these incidental actions
something unexpected happened: When the monkey, remaining perfectly
still, saw the food being grasped by an experimenter in a specific way,
some of its motor neurons discharged. Remarkably, these neurons normally
fired when the monkey itself grasped the food in this way. It was as if
the monkey’s brain was directly mirroring the actions it observed. This
“neural resonance,” which was later also demonstrated in
humans, suggested the existence of a special type of "mirror" neurons that help us understand other people’s actions.
Do you find yourself wondering, now, whether a similar mirror mechanism
could have caused your pungent empathic reaction to your friend
maltreating herself with a hammer? A group of scientists led by
Christian Keysers
believed so.
The researchers had their participants watch short movie clips of
people being touched, while using functional magnetic resonance imaging
(fMRI) to record their brain activity. The brain scans revealed that the
somatosensory cortex, a complex of brain regions processing
touch information, was highly active during the movie
presentations—although participants were not being touched at all. As
was later confirmed by
other studies,
this activity strongly resembled the somatosensory response
participants showed when they were actually touched in the same way. A
recent study by
Esther Kuehn and colleagues even found that, during the observation of a
human hand being touched, parts of the somatosensory cortex were
particularly active when (judging by perspective) the hand clearly
belonged to another person.
No comments:
Post a Comment